Schooled in failure?
Fact or myth - teachers favor boys; girls
respond by withdrawing. (research; includes related article on
all-girls' schools)
By Amy Saltzman
Abstract: Research indicates that girls' self-esteem typically
declines in
early adolescence enough to lower their academic performance, but
the extent
and cause of the self-esteem decline are under debate. Research
findings about
girls, self-esteem and academic performance are discussed.
Full Text COPYRIGHT 1994 U.S. News and World Report Inc.Source:
U.S. News & World Report, Nov 7, 1994 v117 n18 p88(5).
Words like "tough" and "spunky" pop out when
Vina Wise talks about the girls
in the ninth-grade English classes she has taught for a decade.
"They aren't
afraid to speak up," s ays the veteran Fayetteville, N.C.,
teacher. "If they
make a mistake, they just keep trying. The boys are much more
worried about
getting something wrong and are less creative." But in Marie
Nolan's 10th- and
11th-grade chemistry classes in suburban Atlanta, "the boys
view chemistry w
ith a sense of adventure and excitement. Most of the girls aren't
even
comfortable lighting Bun sen burners."
Which picture adds up? Recent studies and books leave no apparent
doubt that
school breaks girls' spirit. It starts when boys, who are
typically louder and
more aggressive than girls, devour teachers' attention in the
early-elementary
grades. Girls tend to be well behaved, so they fade into the
background, at
most, praised for their handwriting and neat projects. Buffeted
by years of
such ego pummeling, girls' self-esteem erodes in early
adolescence. They
gradually turn away from subjects like math and science-and from
careers in
those fields.
The source of this dismal depiction was the American Association
of Universit
y Women, whose 1991 report-"Shortchanging Girls,
Shortchanging
America"-claimed to document a drop in girls' self-esteem
in early
adolescence. The following year, "How Schools Shortchange Girls," a study by
the AAUW and the Wellesley College Center for Research on Women,
expanded on
the theme by examining the way classroom dynamics affect
self-esteem. And over
the past year or so, a half-dozen related books have appeared.
In School
Girls: Young Women, Self-Esteem and the Confidence Gap
(Doubleday, $23.50),
published in September, journalist Peggy Orenstein offers an
anecdotal
treatment of the self-esteem crisis through a yearlong
observation of girls in
two middle schools. On the other side, Christina Hoff Sommers,
author of Who
Stole Feminism? (Simon & Schuster, $23), published last
summer, attacks the
credibility of virtually all gender-bias research as itself
biased and
lacking in substance. Sommers, an associate professor of
philosophy at Clark
University in Worcester, Mass., has been on the talk show circuit
and profiled in major newspapers.
The "shortchanging syndrome" quickly attained almost
conventional-wisdom status. Since 1991, applications at girls' schools and women's
colleges have
climbed 37 and 30 percent, respectively. More parents, according
to teachers
and academics, are pushing their daughters to participate in
competitive
sports, attend science camps and join computer clubs to help them
stay
competitive with boys.
But the conventional wisdom is under attack. The studies
supporting it are being challenged, and detractors argue that the growing presence of
women in the
workplace and on campus defies the notion of lagging self-esteem.
More women
than men, they note, are now enrolled in college and graduate
school.
What is myth and what is fact? Would girls be better off
attending single-sex
schools or classes? Or will the debate decay into a blip on the
social science
radar screen? Is self -esteem truly connected to academic success
at all? And
what should parents be doing-if anything? A look at recent
studies, and
interviews with dozens of academics, psychologists, students ,
parents and
teachers, yield the conclusion that girls may indeed have a
self-esteem
problem that could hamper their performance in certain academic
subjects. But
the extent and even the cause have been obscured by zealousness
and sloppy
research. Here are the issues:
Do schools favor boys? A single numerical assertion-that boys in
elementary a
nd middle school call out answers eight times more than girls
do-underpins
much of the pro-bias case. A computer search of major newspapers
and national
magazines turned up at least 29 articles over the past two years
citing the
study that ostensibly drew that conclusion. But the claim has no
basis.
The "8 to 1" ratio originated with Myra and David
Sadker, a husband and wife
team of education professors at American University in
Washington, D.C.,
regarded as pre-eminent in classroom gender issues. They cite
the ratio in
articles, interviews and lectures. When boys call out, report
the Sadkers,
teachers respond by giving them full attention and making
constructive
comments, while girls who call out are usually admonished to
raise their
hands. The couple's work was key to the Wellesley/AAUW report,
and the 8-to-1
ratio is a cornerstone of the Sadkers' latest book, Failing at
Fairness: How
America's Schools Cheat Girls (1994, Charles Scribner's Sons,
$22), which
documents how girls are treated differently in the classroom.
The Wellesley/AAUW report that popularized the figure, however,
misinterpreted a 1981 article by the Sadkers in an education journal called
The Pointer
(since renamed Preventing School Failure). The article actually
states that
"boys, particularly low-achieving boys, receive 8 to 10
times as many
reprimands as do their female counterparts." "Calling
out" is not mentioned.
Moreover, the Sadkers now admit that even the raw number they
cited may have
been flawed. According to them, it came from a pilot study of
public and
private classes in Washington, D.C., conducted at the end of the
school year
mostly in low-income parts of the city-factors that David Sadker
now says
could have produced an unusually rowdy atmosphere. The findings,
says Sadker,
were presented in an unpublished paper at a symposium sponsored
by the
American Educational Research Association, a Washington,
D.C.-based group that
provides resources for educational researchers. The Sadkers no
longer have a
copy of the paper, and AERA could not track it down.
Subsequent studies have concluded that the ratio of boys to girls
who call out questions and answers is much lower than 8 to 1. In a 1990
study of 1,332
North Carolina students in ninth-grade physical science and
11th-grade
chemistry, Gail Jones, an associate professor of education at the
University
of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, found that boys called out
twice as often
as girls did. Jones found no evidence that teachers were likelier
to scold
girls, or otherwise respond differently, when they called out.
She did find
that boys were praised over the course of the day about twice as
often as
girls were. She doesn't dismiss the possibility of bias but
believes a major
reason for the difference may be that boys called out more often
and consequently had more opportunities to be praised.
Many experts now believe that it is the misbehavior of boys and
the methods teachers use to control them, rather than a pervasive bias against
girls, that
may gradually eat away at girls' self-esteem. "Teachers pay
more attention to
boys because they act out more, not because of some widespread
sexism," says
Jere Brophy, a professor of teacher education at Michigan State
University
who has studied teachers and their students for more than 20
years. Educators
need to switch their focus, he says, from trying to eradicate
sexism directed
against girls to training teachers to better discipline boys.
Does girls' self-esteem suffer more in early adolescence?
Probably, although
it depends on what is being measured. Most research on adolescent
self-esteem
does support the AAUW 's findings of a significant slide for
girls during
adolescence-but much of it relates to physical appearance.
Studies by Susan
Harter, a professor of psychology at the University of Denver,
are typical in
this regard. She has found that boys and girls feel equally
positive about
their physical appearance in third grade, but every year
thereafter, girls'
opinion of their looks drops; boy s' stays about the same.
Surveys that assess girls' and boys' confidence in their academic
ability are
less conclusive. The gap between the sexes is narrow enough, in
fact, to be
breached by the well-stud ied tendency of boys to brag more than
girls. And in
one of the handful of recent studies that examined actual
behavior rather
than relying on students' opinions, girls had a better image of
themselves as
students than boys did. The 1993 study, of 400 North Carolina
junior high
school students, found that more girls than boys answered
"very often" or
"fairly often" to such statements as "I offer to
speak in front of the class"
and "I speak up for my own ideas." Study author William Purkey, a professor
of education at the University of North Carolina at Greensboro,
got similar
results when he asked the students' teachers the same questions.
Christina Hoff Sommers accuses the AAUW of using "deceptive
figures" to exaggerate the drop in girls' self-esteem. She notes that the AAUW's
summary of its
1991 self-esteem study includes only the 29 percent of high
school girls and
the 46 percent of high school boys who responded "always
true" to the
statement: "I'm happy the way I am." If the AAUW had
listed all positive
responses, says Sommers, including "sort of true" and
"sometimes
true/sometimes false," the totals would have added up to a
nearly identical 88
percent for girls and 92 per cent for boys.
But that would have been bad science. In the first place,
standard practice i
n such a survey is to report responses only at the extremes,
which are
considered more valid than thos e in the nebulous middle.
Moreover, the AAUW
took an additional step. Each response was weighted, from 5 for
"always true"
to 1 for "always false," then statistically combined
and averaged. The results
echoed the raw survey findings-in self-esteem, girls still came
out
considerably behind boys.
Is self-esteem tied to academic success? If there is a
connection, it is elus
ive. The AAUW's 1991 report noted that the highest self-esteem
was found among
African-American girls and boys, considered among the groups most
at-risk
academically.
The AAUW says the sample of young black males was too small to
allow conclusi
ons. But the phenomenon of higher self-esteem among black girls
seems genuine,
says Janie Vic toria Ward, an associate professor of education
and human
services at Simmons College in Boston , who has studied black
girls
extensively and who served on the academic panel that review ed
the AAUW
findings. She believes the black culture's emphasis on
independence and
assertiv eness gives black girls a greater sense of self-worth
than that of
white girls. As a result, they aren't as likely to be beaten down
by concerns
over physical appearance.
Ward has also found that black girls maintain high self-esteem by
placing les
s emphasis on areas where they don't feel competent-namely,
school. Yet on
measures of academic self -esteem, as reported in the AAUW
survey, a larger
percentage of black girls than of white bo ys or girls agreed
strongly with
such statements as "I'm proud of the work I do in
school" a nd "My teacher is
proud of me." In fact, the group with the lowest academic
self-esteem-white
girl s-outperforms virtually every other group, including white
boys, in most
academic areas, makin g for a gossamer-thin connection between
self-esteem and
academic success.
Several studies have shown that girls consistently get grades
equal to or bet
ter than boys' from elementary school through college. Even in
math and
science, boys no longer outs hine girls by that much. On the
National
Assessment of Educational Progress test, last given i n 1992,
17-year-old boys
outperformed the girls by a barely significant 4 points in math .
In science,
the boys were 10 points higher, but that compared with 17 points
in 1982. And
girls bettered boys by 12 points in reading proficiency and 17
points in
writing proficiency.
The Scholastic Assessment Test remains an Achilles heel for
girls. In 1994, h
igh school males had an insignificant 4-point advantage in the
verbal portion
of the SAT but a 41 -point bulge in math-a difference that
translates into
more National Merit scholars and more sch olarship money.
Numerous groups have
suggested that the SAT's reliance on multiple-choice questi ons
favors boys,
who seem to be more comfortable making guesses. Several studies
support th is
contention, although the Educational Testing Service, which
created the test,
claims the for mat is fair to both sexes. Nonetheless, the
results of ETS's
own follow-up studies would seem to ind icate a bias against
females. One such
recent study found that women, who score an average of 33 points
less than men
do on the math portion of the SATs, do no worse in college math
courses .
Does self-esteem affect career choice? Echoing other studies, the
AAUW report
found that girls say they enjoy math in earlier grades but tend
to shy away in
adolescence. That may be changing. As many girls as boys now take
advanced
math classes in high school and major in math almost as often in
college. At
the graduate level, however, the number of women entering m ath
programs drops
off significantly. Just 21 percent of math doctorates and 39
percent of ma
ster's degrees went to women in 1992, according to the Department
of
Education. The vast majori ty of women still flock to
traditionally
lower-paying fields such as English literature, psy chology,
communications
and education; men dominate higher-paying areas like engineering
and science.
Despite great progress for women, 60 percent of law, medical and
dental
students are men.
Could self-esteem problems be partly to blame for differences in
education an
d career choice? In the end, the studies and research have no
answer-and
without answers, social sci ence cannot offer workable, long-term
solutions.
That responsibility ultimately falls to parents, who will need to
look at
their own assumptions and the needs of their daughters as
individuals-an d
then decide what course makes the most sense for them.
RELATED ARTICLE: Emulating all-girls' schools
Are there ways to encourage girls' interest in math and science?
The obvious
opt ion is an all-female school. Graduates of single-sex schools
and colleges
tend to score hi gher on tests, major in math and science, earn
postgraduate
degrees and get higher pay. Unfortu nately, the entire country
has only 84
women's colleges and a few hundred girls' schools, an d tuition
can be hefty.
But there are alternatives:
Single-sex classes. A handful of experiments with all-girls math
and science
classes show at least a short-term benefit. Most schools have
gotten around
potential legal challenges by officially opening the classes to
boys-while
letting them know the classes are aimed at gir ls. An all-girls
Algebra 2
class last year at Ventura High School in California led to a
record s ign-up
by girls for trigonometry, the next level-51 this fall compared
with 28 last
year. Teaching m ethods used in all-girl settings also can be
incorporated.
Studies show that seating students i n small groups encourages
cooperative
learning and cuts down on the likelihood that a few boist erous
boys will
dominate discussions.
Promote math, science and computer clubs for girls. Clubs give
girls a social
network that boys who play computer games and do science
experiments together
in the basement alre ady enjoy. The trick is getting them to
join. When Jayne
Kasten, a former computer teacher at a St. Louis County middle
school, wanted
to lure girls to a computer club called Female Elec tronic
Marvels, she asked
a few who were leaders to come and bring some friends. Within
three mo nths,
more than 30 girls had joined. They take field trips to see women
in technical
career s and experiment with interior design software and other
programs to
boost their interest in comp uters.
Verbal girls can be good at math, too. Parents and teachers often
assume that
a child with good language and writing skills won't do as well
with numbers.
Such thinking is base less. When Patricia Campbell's daughter,
Kathryn, was in
sixth grade, the school resisted p lacing her in advanced math
even though she
did well in math. Her teachers felt she should foc us on her
superior language
skills. But Campbell, an education consultant in Groton, Mass. ,
persisted.
Kathryn took the advanced class. Now 17, she is majoring in math
and
linguistics at the University of Chicago.
Parents worried about passing on a math phobia should read the
1993 edition o
f the Sheila Tobias classic, Overcoming Math Anxiety (Norton,
$23). The book
debunks myths ab out math and gender and offers tips on
recognizing and
defanging math sexism.
Books and magazines can break down gender barriers. Even toddlers
can get the message from books like Byron Barton's Machines at
Work (Crowell, $15), which features bright ly colored
illustrations of both sexes toiling on a
construction site. For preschoolers, an example of a book that
portrays a
gutsy girl is Amazing Grace (Dial, $14), the tale of a self-conf
ident black
girl who becomes anything she wants to by acting out
adventuresome roles from books and s tories. New Moon magazine
($25 for six issues a year, PO Box 3587, Duluth, MN 55803) gives
g irls from 8 to 14 a stream of positive female images and role
models in areas like sports an d science.
Talk up sports and after-school programs. Team sports offer
obvious benefits. But any activity in which physical appearance
is not emphasized, from cleaning up a local stream to joining the
Girl Scouts, will help girls feel good even if they don't look
like models in Sa ssy.